Asian Communication Research
[ Special Issue: Hidden Gems in Communication Studies ]
Asian Communication Research - Vol. 20, No. 1, pp.27-32
ISSN: 1738-2084 (Print) 2765-3390 (Online)
Print publication date 30 Apr 2023
Received 11 Jan 2023 Accepted 27 Jan 2023
DOI: https://doi.org/10.20879/acr.2023.20.005

Donohue et al.’s (1983) Hidden Gem: Research on the Interchange of Verbal and Nonverbal Cues to Immediacy Unlocked Relational Communication, Offline and Online

Joseph B. Walther
Center for Information Technology and Society, Department of Communication, University of California, Santa Barbara, USA

Correspondence to: Joseph B. Walther Department of Communication, University of California Santa Barbara, 4007 SSMS Building, MC 4020, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA Email: jwalther@ucsb.edu

Copyright ⓒ 2023 by the Korean Society for Journalism and Communication Studies

One hidden gem of a paper is more hidden than most, in that it was never actually published. However, Donohue et al.’s (1983/2023) International Communication Association conference paper, “The Effects of Distance Violations on Verbal Immediacy,” empirically confirmed an incredibly important and fundamental principle of communication. The principle has to do with the functional relationship between nonverbal and verbal cue systems in the communication of “relational messages of affinity or dislike” (Donohue et al., 1983/2023, p. 1). Although it is simple, the principle was antithetical to the dominant assumptions of the nonverbal communication research literature at the time. The principle was also quite contrary to the assumptions and assertions soon to have appeared about the roles and functions of nonverbal versus verbal messages in (what would emerge as) the field of computer-mediated communication.

The principle that the research demonstrated was that nonverbal and verbal/linguistic cues are functionally interchangeable and translatable for communicating interpersonal immediacy. That is, people can signal changes in immediacy to one another—their degree of affinity and liking—not just through their physical, nonverbal behaviors but also through systematic variations in the linguistic aspects of the words they say. The two systems, nonverbal and verbal, work in tandem. They can do the same job. If one of these systems is unavailable, people use the other system to make up for it.

This idea may appear intuitive to look at now. But intuition is a funny thing when it comes to nonverbal and verbal relational communication: People are frequently unaware about their own behavior of this type (for review, see Knapp et al., 1978); they “do not appear to consciously choose to use some type of immediacy … These variables remain as a seen but unnoticed feature of the interaction,” according to Donohue et al. (1983/2023, p. 19). Thus, people rely on retrospective intuitions about it, some of which are stereotypical yet unreliable (e.g., gaze aversion during deception; Global Deception Research Team, 2006). Presumptions about the utility of nonverbal and verbal cues for relational communication then, as now, are often met with skepticism about the principle that Donohue et al. (1983/2023) established, by scholars and by others.

The following work describes the dominant thinking about verbal and nonverbal channel reliance at the time Donoghue et al.’s (1983/2023) paper was written, and the genesis of their alternative position. Following a very brief summary of the experiment and results, the discussion returns to the breakthroughs this study established and the immense contributions of this study in both face to face and computer-mediated communication scholarship.

The Roles of Nonverbal and Verbal Communication

One focus within interpersonal and nonverbal communication research has been called the study of channel reliance. It seeks to determine and explain which among the variety of nonverbal and verbal cues that might be used, do people tend to use, when and for what communicative purposes. This literature has been strongly affected by such classic positions as Watzlawick et al. (1967) The Pragmatics of Communication, that asserted there are report and command functions that accompany each other in any interpersonal exchange. Report is the content or substance or topic of an exchange, whereas the command is the relational aspect by which communicators signal dominance, affection, etc. According to Watzlawick et al. (and many others), report is communicated by verbal content, whereas the commend is signaled through nonverbal means. This position is often echoed, couched in different terms such as the task and maintenance dimensions of communication, task and relational dimensions, etc., and the intuitive assessment that nonverbal communication is best, most natural, or generally relied upon, for non-task relational messages.

The literature generally characterizes nonverbal communication as both innately and uniquely capable of conveying relational messages, whereas verbal communication was not. Nonverbal cues are “implicitly seen as natural or even ‘sole’ carriers of relational information, (and) subtle verbal variations that also carry relational information have been neglected,” according to Donohue et al. (1983/2023, p. 3). Empirical research seemed to support the notion that nonverbal communication had a monopoly on relational communication: Mehrabian and Ferris (1967) concluded (without recognizing a methodological error) that 93% of the social meaning of spoken utterances derive from nonverbal variations in kinesics and vocalics; only 7%, they claimed, emanated from words. Argyle and Dean’s (1965) equilibrium theory experiments showed that, when one stranger (an experimental confederate) came close to naïve participants, participants backed up. When they could not back up, participants averted eye contact in response to the invasion of space. This showed that nonverbal cue systems—proxemics and gaze in this case—were functionally interchangeable immediacy cues. Language was not part of that equation.

Research on language, at the time, mostly focused on content and fluency effects, according to Donohue et al. (1983/2023). However, their literature review revealed some countervailing positions asserting language variations’ connotation of affinity and other aspects of relational communication. These included Bradac et al.’s (1979) review of primary studies examining the lexical variables intensity, immediacy, and diversity; and Weiner and Mehrabian’s (1968) proposal that certain linguistic variations constitute verbal immediacy.

The Challenge and its Demonstration

Donohue et al.’s (1983/2023) paper challenged both the equilibrium theoretic principle as well as its incorporation of nonverbal channels exclusively. Burgoon (1978) had by this time articulated her Communicative Model of Violations of Distancing Expectations, (Burgoon et al., 1979) later to become known as nonverbal expectancy violations theory. Burgoon’s theory accounts for discrepancies that equilibrium theory, and other immediacy exchange theories, could not: Why it is that we back off from people who come very close sometimes, but, when someone is attracted to the too-close individual, they like it and reciprocate it. (If this exception to equilibrium theory’s predicted retreat from any proxemic invasion did not exist, after all, the human species could not procreate!) Burgoon’s elegant solution was to conceptualize proxemics as being governed by normative expectations, and that responses to proxemic expectancy violations are moderated by one’s appraisal of the violator (see Burgoon, 2016 for a review).

The Donohue et al. (1983/2023) experiment extended Burgoon’s theory beyond proxemics, to incorporate another form of immediacy. Not only that: The specific other form, in this case, was verbal rather than nonverbal. The experiment examined whether spatial violations toward (or away from) an individual who could not retreat from (or approach) the violator would prompt commensurate “psychological distance displayed in verbal cues” (Donohue et al., 1983/2023, p. 7). Hypotheses predicted that if a violator came unexpectedly close, and was rewarding, target persons would reciprocate immediacy verbally; but if the violator was unrewarding, the target would express nonimmediacy verbally. If a rewarding individually violated proxemic expectations retreating, the target would chase them through greater verbal immediacy.

By and large, the results of the experiment supported the most central hypothesized patterns, with one exception. Using student subjects, the experiment attempted to implement systematic variation in violators’ rewardingness by employing either a real friend of the target, or a random stranger, to enact distance violations. This manipulation was unsuccessful. Students, apparently, like other students, at least at first, and all targets responded to all violators in the hypothesized high reward patterns: More verbal immediacy when the violator either approached or retreated, compared to no-violation control conditions. Two of the four dimensions of verbal immediacy reflected hypothesized effects, and two others did not.

Impact in Face to Face and Mediated Communication Research

Despite the mixed support, these findings were breakthroughs. As many readers will know, expectancy violations theory would grow in scope and precision. Its approach to conceptualizing and operationalizing violator’s reward value would be refined by Burgoon and colleagues and many others. Most importantly, for present purposes, this study demonstrated the theory’s incredible heuristic value. It opened the door to consider all forms of immediacy behavior—verbal, nonverbal, and otherwise (later formalized in Burgoon & Hale, 1988). It established the principle that immediacy takes many forms in many channels and that these forms and channels can be functionally interchangeable in their expression of relational communication.

Not only was this benchmark important in the domain of face to face interpersonal interaction. The conceptual and empirical precedent set by Donohue et al. (1983/2023) provided a cornerstone for the development of a particular second-generation theory in computer-mediated communication (CMC), as well. CMC at that time supported only typewritten verbal messages. One of the central questions in the field was how the medium’s occlusion of nonverbal cues affected communication. Several first-generation CMC theories addressed this question drawing directly from the channel reliance literature in nonverbal communication research. Citing psychological studies of nonverbal interaction (e.g., Ekman et al., 1980) they proposed that, because of its absence of nonverbal cues, CMC would be inferior to face to face communication in its positive relational qualities. Numerous experiments, from the 1970s through the 1990s and beyond, supported these assertions (although the validity of many such studies were challenged in the ‘90s; see Walther, 2011 for a review).

An alternative, second-generation CMC theory drew directly on the principle of interchangeability between nonverbal and verbal cues of affinity that Donohue et al. (1983/2023) established. The social information processing theory of CMC (Walther, 1992) makes that principle an explicit assumption within its own theoretical framework: “Relational messages are transmitted (i.e., encoded and decoded) by nonverbal and/or verbal, linguistic, and textual manipulations” (p. 69; Assumption #4), including “the words and other written matter that appear as typed characters transmitted in CMC … verbal content, lexical variation, syntactic usage, or other feature of language that may be conveyed in written communication” (p. 82). The theory contends that individuals, in whatever setting, exploit whatever cues are available to communicate content and relational communication; but when (as in the case of text-based CMC) there are no nonverbal cues available, communicators translate the relational messages (that may otherwise appear nonverbally), into verbal content and style variations (see Westerman et al., 2008). The theory can be thought of as a thought experiment applying Donohue et al. to CMC: If one student increases verbal immediacy when another presumably likeable student physically retreats, as Donohue et al. found, will the same thing happen when that second student retreats all the way behind a computer? Given ample opportunity to exchange of immediacy cues verbally, tests of the new theory found, the answer was yes.

Social information processing theoretic research addressed a variety of cue systems and their application a variety of online phenomena. Cue systems came to include chronemics (Kalman & Rafaeli, 2010), irony (Hancock, 2004), emoticons (Derks et al., 2007), and self-disclosure and personal questions (Tidwell & Walther, 2002), among others. One study in particular demonstrated the translatability between nonverbal and verbal expressions of liking between parallel dyadic face -to-face and computer chat conversations (Walther et al., 2005): Face to face partners relied primarily on a specific group of kinesic cues and secondarily on certain vocalic cues, whereas CMC partners relied on a set of verbal cues and strategies (and a couple emoticons) to express liking. Although the sets of cues were different, there was no significant difference in the amount of affinity detected between the two settings. The principles helped illuminate how groups using plain-text electronic messages developed trust in distributed collaborations (Wilson et al., 2006), to online romances and internet date finding (Gibbs et al., 2006).

It is essentially because of its subsumption of the interchangeability heuristic from Donohue et al. (1983/2023) that the social information processing theory of CMC generated its own influence, and years after its publication it “stood the test of time and has become a stimulus for new conceptualizations of communication phenomena” (National Communication Association, n.d.).

The Donohue at al.’s (1983/2023) paper comes from a time when the field was actively exploring and debating questions about how communication works; how alternative symbols create messages. It tested a scientifically creative and courageous model, taking a counterintuitive position that was at odds with the dominant literature, identifying the logic that allowed it (or required it) nevertheless to be true, and demonstrating it in a clear and replicable way. The resonance and magnitude of its simple principle of symbolic multiplicity and interchangeability expanded the way researchers could see, hear, and read how people influence one another’s fundamental relational dynamics across contexts as well as across technologies unforeseen by the paper’s authors.

I was fortunate that Professor Burgoon gave me a copy of this paper in the 1980s when I studied nonverbal communication under her tutelage. Professor Gavriel Salomon also gave me empirical-based articles about computer-mediated communication to read, saying “Here’s what happens to interpersonal communication when there’s no nonverbal cues.” They demonstrated significantly less affinity in CMC because, they asserted, the expression of affinity requires nonverbal communication. Their ontology and that of Donohue et al. (1983/2023) seemed mutually exclusive. The inconsistency demanded articulation of theoretical and methodological explanations that accounted for when and why each competing position could appear to be correct. Social information processing theory was an answer to that question, allowed entirely by the principle of interchangeability established by the heuristic vision of Donohue et al.’s pioneering study.

Acknowledgments

The author thanks America Edwards for her assistance in preparing this work, and Professors Burgoon and Donohue for their cooperation and inspiration.

References

  • Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28(3), 289–304. [https://doi.org/10.2307/2786027]
  • Bradac, J. J., Bowers, J. W., & Courtright, J. A. (1979). Three language variables in communication research: Intensity, immediacy, and diversity. Human Communication Research, 5(3), 257–269. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1979.tb00639.x]
  • Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model of personal space violations: Explication and an initial test. Human Communication Research, 4(2), 129–142. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1978.tb00603.x]
  • Burgoon, J. K. (2016). Expectancy violations theory. In C. R. Berger & M. E. Roloff (Eds.), The international encyclopedia of interpersonal communication (pp. 1–9). John Wiley & Sons. [https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118540190.wbeic102]
  • Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1988). Nonverbal expectancy violations: Model elaboration and application to immediacy behaviors. Communication Monographs, 55(1), 58–79. [https://doi.org/10.1080/03637758809376158]
  • Burgoon, J. K., Stacks, D. W., & Woodall, W. G. (1979). A communicative model of violations of distancing expectations. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 43(2), 153–167. [https://doi.org/10.1080/10570317909373963]
  • Derks, D., Bos, A. E. R., & von Grumbkow, J. (2007). Emoticons and social interaction on the Internet: The importance of social context. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(1), 842–849. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2004.11.013]
  • Donohue, W. A., Diez, M. E., Stahle, R., & Burgoon, J. K. (2023). The effects of distance violations on verbal immediacy. SocArXiv. (Reprinted from "The effects of distance violations on verbal immediacy: An exploration" [Paper presentation], May 1983, International Communication Association Annual Meeting, Dallas, TX, United States.) [https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/t2yzq]
  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., & Scherer, K. (1980). Relative importance of face, body, and speech in judgments of personality and affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(2), 270–277. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.38.2.270]
  • Gibbs, J. L., Ellison, N. B., & Heino, R. D. (2006). Self-presentation in online personals: The role of anticipated future interaction, self-disclosure, and perceived success in Internet dating. Communication Research, 33(2), 152–177. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650205285368]
  • Global Deception Research Team (2006). A world of lies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37(1), 60–74. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022105282295]
  • Hancock, J. T. (2004). Verbal irony use in face-to-face and computer-mediated conversations. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 23(4), 447–463. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X04269587]
  • Kalman, Y. M., & Rafaeli, S. (2010). Online pauses and silence: Chronemic expectancy violations in written computer-mediated communication. Communication Research, 38(1), 54–69. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650210378229]
  • Knapp, M. L., Wiemann, J. M., & Daly, J. A. (1978). Nonverbal communication: Issues and appraisal. Human Communication Research, 4(3), 271–280. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1978.tb00616.x]
  • Mehrabian, A., & Ferris, S. R. (1967). Inference of attitudes from nonverbal communication in two channels. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31(3), 248–252. [https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024648]
  • National Communication Association. (n.d.). Charles H. Woolbert Research Award. https://www.natcom.org/awards/charles-h-woolbert-research-award
  • Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. B. (2002). Computer-mediated communication effects on disclosure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations: Getting to know one another a bit at a time. Human Communication Research, 28(3), 317–348. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00811.x]
  • Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A relational perspective. Communication Research, 19(1), 52–90. [https://doi.org/10.1177/009365092019001003]
  • Walther, J. B. (2011). Theories of computer-mediated communication and interpersonal relations. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), The handbook of interpersonal communication (4th ed., pp. 443–479). Sage.
  • Walther, J. B., Loh, T., & Granka, L. (2005). Let me count the ways: The interchange of verbal and nonverbal cues in computer-mediated and face-to-face affinity. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24(1), 36–65. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X04273036]
  • Watzlawick, P., Bavelas, J. B., & Jackson, D. D. (1967). Pragmatics of human communication: Patterns pathologies, and paradoxes. W. W. Norton & Company.
  • Weiner, M., & Mehrabian, A. (1968). Language within language: Immediacy, a channel in verbal communication. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Westerman, D., Van Der Heide, B., Klein, K. A., & Walther, J. B. (2008). How do people really seek information about others? Information seeking across Internet and traditional communication channels. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13(3), 751–767. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2008.00418.x]
  • Wilson, J. M., Straus, S. G., & McEvily, B. (2006). All in due time: The development of trust in computer-mediated and face-to-face teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99(1), 16–33. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.08.001]