Asian Communication Research
[ Special Issue: Hidden Gems in Media Studies ]
Asian Communication Research - Vol. 21, No. 1, pp.3-3
ISSN: 1738-2084 (Print) 2765-3390 (Online)
Print publication date 30 Apr 2024
Received 31 Dec 2023 Revised 23 Jan 2024 Accepted 30 Jan 2024
DOI: https://doi.org/10.20879/acr.2024.21.004

Implications of Shifts in Dominant Mediums on Media-Induced Feelings of Connectedness

Mary Beth Oliver1 ; Yansheng Liu1 ; Rebecca N. H. de Leeuw2 ; Arthur A. Raney3 ; Enny Das4 ; Marie-Louise Mares5
1Bellisario College of Communications, Pennsylvania State University, USA
2Communication & Media, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Netherlands
3Department of Communication, University at Buffalo, USA
4Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Netherlands
5Department of Communication Arts, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA

Correspondence to: Mary Beth OliverBellisario College of Communications, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA Email: mbo@psu.edu

Copyright ⓒ 2024 by the Korean Society for Journalism and Communication Studies

Abstract

This essay focuses on what dominant mediums in a culture imply about feelings of connectedness with others. Drawing on Joshua Meyrowitz’s book No Sense of Place, we argue that when dominant mediums shift in our cultures, how we consume media content—with and about others—also shifts. We focus on two factors related to connectedness: cognitive empathy and synchrony, and we outline how changing medium-related landscapes may affect each factor. Finally, we encourage scholars of positive media psychology to consider both media content and dominant mediums in future research on uplifting, self-transcendent experiences.

Keywords:

positive media psychology, medium theory, empathy, synchrony

It may seem odd to identify a scholarly work as a “hidden gem” when Google Scholar reports it as having over 8000 citations. Joshua Meyrowitz’s No Sense of Place: The Impact of Electronic Media on Social Behavior was published in 1985. This work garnered countless positive reviews and was the recipient of the 2014 International Communication Association’s Fellows Book Award and the 1986 National Association of Broadcaster’s award for Best Book on Electronic Media. Why, then, might we focus on this particular work as a hidden gem? We selected this work as scholars of positive media psychology who believe that not only was Meyrowitz prophetic in his scholarship, but that he also paves new avenues for thinking about and researching how media function to affect our sense of connectedness with others. In this article, we first provide an overview of Meyrowitz’s arguments. We then we apply this literature to two concepts related to connectedness: cognitive empathy and synchrony. We end by discussing ways that our shifting dominant mediums may affect our sense of connectedness, both in terms of fragmentation but also in terms of heightening affiliation with broader and more geographically diverse groups.


A BRIEF SUMMARY OF MEYROWITZ’S NO SENSE OF PLACE

As a medium theorist, Meyrowtiz provides a sweeping overview of how dominant forms of communication serve to shape and sustain social behavior, enabling individuals to convincingly perform social roles or, in contrast, undermining their performances to make them seem inauthentic, if not contemptable. Dominant mediums of communication also provide differential access to information, allowing some individuals the resources and abilities to engage with “more” information, while also preventing access among individuals who have fewer resources. As a result, Meyrowitz argues that dominant mediums not only shape social behavior in terms of the types of information that we communicate, but also in terms of the extent to which social groups share the same information.

In his work, Meyrowitz draws heavily on historians, sociologists, media scholars, and communication researchers to discuss our social lives. He starts by examining how communication likely functioned throughout history— from oral cultures to print-based cultures and then to a television-based culture. When communication took the form of oral exchanges only, people likely formed ties with others in their close communities. Unless a member of a community was able to travel far from home, societies based on oral-based communication likely had very little contact with, and hence knowledge about, other societies that were geographically distant. As print became the dominant medium, access to information and the spread of information also shifted. Written language requires specific skills, namely literacy, to encode and decode. Consequently, members of a community who have not developed literacy skills (e.g., children) or have been prevented from developing literacy skills (e.g., enslaved people) have unequal access to information. At the same time, though, written communication can be preserved and can be transported more easily than oral communication, drawing people from other cultures together through shared information. As a result, print-based cultures were more likely to be hierarchical by allowing some members of the culture greater access to information, but at the same time, they were better able to communicate with other, distant cultures.

The advent of television ushered in a radical shift in access to information and, hence, social structure and social behaviors. First, the skills required to consume information from television are much less difficult than those for print. Television messages also differ from print in terms of the effort they require to utilize— Print messages are often objects (e.g., books, newspapers) that must be purchased, obtained, or physically transported, whereas television messages can be obtained through the push of a button. Finally, unlike print that is often targeted to specific audiences (e.g., children’s books, gendered publications such as Ladies Home Journal), television is frequently designed to appeal to the largest audience possible. As a result, it is not unusual that otherwise diverse audiences (e.g., children and adults) consume the same media messages. Together, the ease of access to television coupled with the industry’s efforts to attract all audiences serves to make access to information explicit. That is, the prevalence of and ease of access to information not only provides individuals with knowledge about the information transmitted, but it also provides individuals with knowledge about what other people know. As Meyrowitz described, even “taboo” topics that are presented on television can become fodder for public discussion because of the awareness that other people are familiar with the same content as well: “… everyone knows that everyone knows, and everyone knows that everyone knows that everyone knows” (p. 92).

To summarize, No Sense of Place describes the importance of dominant mediums in shaping our social behaviors. As the medium evolves or changes, so do social behaviors and cultures. In the next section we turn to a discussion of our ongoing changes in the media landscape from television to digital-based cultures. Subsequently, we turn to the scholarship of positive media psychology, noting two specific concepts that may play particularly consequential roles in how this shift in dominant mediums affects feeling of connectedness with others.


SHIFTING MEDIUMS, SHIFTING AUDIENCE CONDITIONS

In the age of television—and particularly prior to cable and to recording devices—media consumption often involved not only consuming the same (or very similar) content, but also consuming it at the same time. In the U.S., many households would tune into Walter Cronkite delivering the news, children (and adults) would watch The Wonderful World of Disney on Sunday evenings, and large segments of the viewing public would eagerly await the latest episode of popular television programs such as Gun Smoke or The Mary Tyler Moore Show.

As a reflection of shared and simultaneous viewing, it is perhaps not surprising that in discussing various indicators of audience activity, Levy and Windahl (1985) describe how individuals can utilize media in their social interactions. For example, using media as a “coin of exchange” refers to pre-exposure conversations about upcoming media offerings (e.g., “Are you going to watch NYPD Blue tonight?”). We note, though, that this type of audience activity reflects that individuals can assume common knowledge of media content and can therefore use that common knowledge as the basis for conversation. In contrast, today’s media landscape is so vast and diverse that it is difficult, if not nonsensical, to have such conversations at present (e.g., “Are you going to watch that TikTok video tonight?”).

Of course, we are not arguing that people no longer have any shared and simultaneous media experiences. For example, Dayan and Katz’s (1992) work on “media events” highlights the importance of wide scale, preplanned, nonroutine television coverage of major public events, including occasions such as royal weddings and the moon landing. In today’s landscape, shared and simultaneous media consumption can often be observed for sporting events, live music, and during times of crisis (among others).

With these notable exceptions in mind, the vastly enlarged landscape of media content and delivery options mean that audiences now have the option of selecting content that appeals to their own particular interests and that can be consumed at any time that audiences find convenient. Social media and streaming services are perhaps most notable in this phenomenon, and particularly among younger viewers. For example, a recent U.S. national survey found that whereas 55% of older respondents (Generation X, Boomers, and Matures) reported watching TV shows or movies as their most enjoyed media entertainment activity, that number dropped to only 30% of younger respondents (Generation Z and Millennials) (Lebow, 2022). In contrast, 38% of younger respondents (compared to 12% of older respondents) preferred playing video games and watching user-generated content. Indeed, although traditional TV viewing has declined among all age groups in the U.S., Gen Z populations now consume more entertainment from social platforms than from television, with YouTube and TikTok the most popular. Of course, entertainment is far from being the only type of content undergoing substantial changes. Newspapers, and local newspapers in particular, have suffered massive downturns (Masta & Worden, 2022), with a growing proportion of U.S. adults now reporting that they sometimes (29%) or often (56%) get their news from digital devices (Pew Research Center, 2023).

Before moving to our discussion of the implications of these wide-scale medium changes on feelings of connectedness, it is important to note that Meyrowitz also recognized that in digital-media landscapes, the boundaries that define “us” versus “them” have shifted (Meyrowitz, 1997). In particular, though we may now have more global access to the same information, within our own communities and homes, we now live in frequently disjointed informational spheres.

Even in our homes, experience is splintered: A son may walk around the house with a Walkman, a daughter’s eyes may dart between a parent’s face and a computer screen, and a spouse may leave an intimate embrace to find out what just came in over the home fax machine. More than ever before, therefore, the postmodern, electronic era is one in which everyone else, foreigner or family member, seems somewhat familiar-and somewhat strange. (p. 68)

In sum, Meyrowitz’s (1985) scholarship highlights the importance of mediums in shaping our social roles and the information that is shared among members of a culture. We are now in the midst of a digital culture that, unlike our previous television-based culture, involves limitless media choices and diminished shared consumption within our own communities. With this backdrop in mind, the next section provides a brief overview of research growing out of positive media psychology, including two concepts related to feelings of connectedness that may be particularly relevant to shifting dominant mediums.


POSITIVE MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY AND FEELINGS OF CONNECTEDNESS

Media effects research has historically often focused on potential harms of media exposure such as violence, health-related problems, and stereotyping. However, over the last decade we have seen more scholars recognizing the potential of media to provide users with meaningful, if not self-transcendent experiences that are associated with a host of prosocial outcomes (Raney et al., 2021).

A Brief Overview of Research in Positive Media Psychology

Most people can easily recall being moved by a film, inspired by a piece of music, or touched by an uplifting video featuring love and kindness. However, these types of responses were rarely studied by media effects scholars until relatively recently. Perhaps the general lack of early attention reflects that the concepts of being moved, inspired, or uplifted are somewhat fuzzy and difficult to quantify. Nevertheless, we believe that they are not only generally universal (see, Zickfeld et al., 2019 for example), but that they are particularly powerful in inducing positive outcomes.

Some of the early research in positive media psychology was in response to the seeming paradoxes implied by prevailing theorizing regarding entertainment enjoyment and media selection. For example, mood management theory (Zillmann, 2000; Zillmann & Bryant, 1985) suggested that hedonistic impulses to alleviate pain and to maximize pleasure predicted individuals’ media selection, even if not consciously recognized by the viewer. Likewise, early dispositional approaches to understanding the appeal of entertainment largely suggested that audience enjoyment was maximized when beloved protagonists prevailed against challenges and/or when evil antagonists were brought to justice (Zillmann & Bryant, 1975, 1986).

Although these foundational theories of enter tainment selection and enjoyment undoubtedly aptly apply to a wide swath of viewing experiences, scholars began to question what these theories imply about beloved films that feature the suffering of protagonists and that frequently elicit sadness (Oliver, 1993). Subsequently, researchers began to examine emotions other than sadness or enjoyment, instead considering audience responses such as “meaningful affect,” appreciation, or eudaimonic (rather than hedonic) gratifications (Oliver, 2008; Oliver & Bartsch, 2010; Oliver & Raney, 2011).

In studying the emotional response that might best represent being moved by meaningful entertainment, many researchers turned to the concept of moral elevation (Haidt, 2003). Elevation is an example of a self-transcendent emotion that is elicited when witnessing others display exemplary actions of moral good such as kindness, compassion, or courage. It is typically associated with mixed affect, physiological responses such as tears or a lump in the throat, and importantly, with heightened motivations to be a better person or to live a more meaningful life (Algoe & Haidt, 2009). Further, as an example of a self-transcendent emotion, elevation involves a decreased emphasis on the self and an increased awareness of others and one’s compassionate connection with others (Stellar et al., 2017; Yaden et al., 2017). Consistent with this conceptualization, researchers have found evidence that viewing meaningful media evokes feelings of elevation, with elevation, in turn, associated with heightened altruism (Schnall et al., 2010) and charitability (Freeman et al., 2009), diminished stereotyping (Krämer et al., 2017), and increased feelings of connectedness with others (Janicke & Oliver, 2017; Oliver et al., 2015).

Two Concepts Related to Feelings of Connectedness

Before turning to the question of what shifting media landscapes may imply about feelings of connectedness, we first highlight two aspects of connection that we believe may be consequential: cognitive empathy and synchrony. Though these concepts have yet to be heavily studied within positive media psychology, their relationship with both human connection and with media consumption may make them particularly relevant.

Cognitive Empathy

The general concept of empathy is multidimensional, with broad definitions suggesting that it represents “…the reactions of one individual to the observed experiences of another” (Davis, 1983, p. 113) or that it means “to share and understand others’ internal states” (Zaki & Ochsner, 2016, p. 871). Although empathy is frequently referred to in affective terms, for our purposes we are particularly interested in cognitive empathy or what some scholars refer to as perspective taking.

The debate around whether empathy can best be characterized as a cognitive or affective experience has amassed considerable scholarly attention (Cuff et al., 2016). In this article, we assume that empathy likely involves both affective and cognitive processes (Davis, 2017), but our focus on the cognitive reflects our interest in how media provide one means by which individuals may be able to engage in accurate perspective taking. We are not suggesting that affective empathy is unimportant. Indeed, we believe that its frequent co-occurrence with affective empathy may illustrate that they represent multiple processes that can arise during the experience and outcomes of an empathic episode (e.g., Davis, 2017), that cognitive empathy may serve as a necessary condition for affective empathy to materialize (e.g., Staub, 1990), or even that cognitive empathy in the absence of affective empathy may be associated with unsavory personality traits (e.g., Machiavellianism, callousness, selfishness; see Smith, 2006). Rather, our focus on cognitive empathy is a function of how it represents accurately understanding another person. How might we know what another person is thinking or feeling? The potential number of variables that help us “know” another person is undoubtedly massive, including shared experiences, awareness of personal histories, or even similarities in tastes and preferences. However, given the centrality of media consumption in the lives of most people, we believe that if we consume the same media and are presented with similar, if not identical, information as those we interact with, we have at least some understanding of what is familiar to them.

Synchrony

Synchrony is the second concept that we believe may be important in our exploration of the implications of medium-related changes on feelings of connectedness. In general, the word synchrony is frequently used to refer to individuals in interpersonal interactions who engage in the same physical movements at the same time (see Bowsher-Murray et al., 2022 for example). These physical movements can include behaviors such as nodding, eye gaze, and gate while walking. Further, external factors such as cultural events, religious rituals, and sports (among many others) may also encourage synchrony via dance, singing, or reading aloud as a group. As McNeill (1995) described, “American football crowds, South African demonstrators, patriotic parades, and religious rituals of every description all draw on the emotional affect of rhythmic movements and gestures” (p. 5).

In addition to referring to interpersonal interactions, synchrony has also been used to refer to broader instances of shared experiences occurring simultaneously. For example, Paladino et al. (2010) argued that sharing the same sensory experience (e.g., a brush lightly touching the face) constitutes synchrony, even though the experience itself is not initiated by either party in the interaction. Likewise, imagined synchrony (e.g., imagining walking with a crowd of people) has been shown to mirror the results of actual, interpersonal, behavioral synchrony (Atherton et al., 2019; Crossey et al., 2021), implying that the effects of synchrony may be more far-reaching than initially suggested.

Of particular importance to the present essay, a large literature has demonstrated a connection between synchrony and feelings of affiliation or connectedness (see Rennung & Göritz, 2016). This type of connection has been observed in a variety of contexts, including in musical contexts (Swarbrick et al., 2019), dancing (Tarr et al., 2016), and cultural and spiritual rituals (Fischer et al., 2013), among many others. Additionally, this body of literature includes many operationalizations of affiliation or connectedness, including greater mentalizing (Baimel et al., 2018; Basile et al., 2022), collective self-esteem (Wlodarczyk et al., 2023), empathy (Levy & Feldman, 2019), and blurring of selfother boundaries (Paladino et al., 2010), among many additional prosocial outcomes (see Rennung & Göritz, 2016).

To summarize, a growing number of scholars are now exploring how uplifting or inspiring media may result in a variety of prosocial outcomes. Connection with others is one of the many outcomes studied. As it relates to dominant mediums, we highlight the importance of both cognitive empathy and synchrony.


IMPLICATIONS OF SHIFTING MEDIUMS ON FEELINGS OF CONNECTEDNESS

Within the area of positive media psychology, much of the research has been generally agnostic to the importance of mediums. Although survey-based scholarship has found that people are frequently inspired by uplifting messages found throughout different mediums, including music, film, social media, and television (Raney et al., 2018), much of the research examining the prosocial outcomes of media messages is experimental. The importance of highlighting experimental approaches is twofold. First, most experimental research is focused on individuals rather than on groups, societies, or cultures. Second, experimental research in media psychology frequently explores audience responses to media messages while holding other, potentially confounding variables constant, including the medium through which the messages are delivered.

In contrast, Meyrowitz’s work is not about particular messages per se, but rather about the mediums through which the messages are disseminated. Further, Meyrowitz’s theorizing is not so much about individuals, but rather about social roles and social relationships. Rather than exploring how a given person responds to a media message, Meyrowitz asks us to take a broader view of the implications of mediums on social interactions reflected in group formation, social hierarchies, and transitions in social roles (e.g., children and adults).

At the risk of overstepping the assumptions and boundary conditions of either research tradition, we rather believe that positive media psychologists would benefit from considering the ramifications of both messages and mediums on both individual audiences and on social roles and cultures. Like Meyrowitz’s observation that the shift from a television-based to a digitalbased culture has disorientated and dislocated social roles that were often tied to locale, we, too, believe that this shift in mediums likely has both positive and potentially negative ramifications on feelings of connectedness—a focus of much research in positive media psychology. Below we focus first on the potential challenges that our shifting media landscape may present. We then end by considering how our newly shaped and rapidly evolving mediums may afford novel opportunities for connectedness that were not previously possible.

Challenges

Perhaps the biggest challenge presented by the shift to digital mediums is that people are increasingly uncertain about others’ media habits. When media outlets were limited, audience choices were limited to a handful of possible options. Consequently, it was relatively easy to accurately predict the messages (e.g., entertainment programs, news, etc.) that others were exposed to. In essence, as Meyrowitz argued, television allowed us to “know” what others know. Insofar as cognitive empathy rests on accurately understanding what other people know or believe, the shift from a televisionbased culture to a digital culture in which media offerings are seemingly endless implies that we have essentially lost one important clue as to what others know or believe. In a televisionbased culture, we might have rightly assumed that individuals in the U.S. were familiar with the hearings regarding Richard Nixon, as these hearings were difficult to avoid when consuming television at the time. In today’s digital world, even knowing that another person consumes a heavy diet of media does not provide the same information regarding what other people may have seen, as the vast number of choices makes prediction difficult, if not impossible.

Of course, we are cognizant of the notable exceptions (e.g., sporting events) to this argument. We also acknowledge that some social groups are built upon shared media content. For example, fans of various musical artists have robust communities online and are therefore likely familiar with much of the same content (see the online community for Taylor Swift’s fans at Reddit.com, https://www.reddit.com/ r/TaylorSwift/; see the online community for BTS’s fans at X.com, https://twitter.com/ BTS_ARMY). Although geographically dispersed, members of these fan-based groups form a community because, at least in part, of their shared media and, therefore, their shared understanding of what other members think about, believe, or enjoy.

One implication of the fragmentation of media diets in this digital age is that groups and shared understandings are now simply different, arising not necessarily among our local communities but rather out of our shared preferences, a point brought up by Meyrowitz (1997) in his discussion of a post-modern age of media. Although this change may not represent either an inherently good or bad thing, we note two ways that it may lead to challenges in feelings of connectedness with others. First, the formation of communities based on shared media preferences may result in echo chambers or filter bubbles that serve to support and enhance in-group homophilia and partisanship (Cinelli et al., 2021; Hobolt et al., 2023). Although the concept of echo chambers has been hotly debated (see Ross Arguedas et al., 2022 for example), a recent national U.S. survey showed that approximately 25% of Democrats and Republicans reported turning to like-minded partisan news, and among those who reported such selective exposure, levels of partisanship were more pronounced (Mitchell et al., 2021). The only news source that attracted both Democrats and Republicans was network television, with cable news, radio news, and newspapers showing much higher levels of partisan audiences.

Second, related to the challenge of echo chambers is what the audience assumes the out-group may be consuming (and therefore knowing). A long history of research on hostile media effects (Vallone et al., 1985) and thirdperson effects (Davison, 1983) has demonstrated that people are not only more likely to perceive media stories as opposed to their own deeply held opinions, but are also more likely to believe that other people are more vulnerable to harmful effects of media than is the self. Couched in terms of diverse and vast media selections, what do these two concepts imply about what audiences assume that “the other” knows? One implication is that even when audiences cannot predict what other people consume (and therefore “know”) from their media diet, they may wrongly assume that others are consuming media that is at odds with their own beliefs (a hostile media effect) and are more affected by “out-group” messages (a third-person effect). As a result, because we cannot be certain of what others are consuming, there may be a tendency to assume the “worst”— a tendency that may lead to lower levels of cognitive empathy and, hence, connectedness.

We end this section by providing a media example illustrating the importance of both shared media and of synchronous exposure. On December 5, 2023, Norman Lear, a television producer and screenwriter, passed away. Lear was responsible for numerous television programs but was perhaps most famous for All in the Family—a program that aired in 1971 and that openly and frankly presented contentious social issues such as racism, war, sexism, and political disagreement. Unlike other programs at the time that typically avoided uncomfortable social issues, All in the Family openly addressed these issues, forcing the audience to grapple with contemporary political and cultural problems. In today’s media landscape, one might predict that large swaths of the viewing public would turn away from such content and instead choose to consume something that would not create any sense of unease, yet at the time, All in the Family became the top-rated show for five years in a row (Brownstein, 2021). Rob Reiner, a primary character in the program, reflected on the importance of the viewing context:

The show aired in the era of appointment viewing, when there were only a handful of TV channels and households across the United States tuned in to the same programs at the same time. The shifting habits of American viewers, who can much more easily silo themselves in echo chambers when it comes to viewing habits, has only contributed to the fracturing and divisions, Reiner said. Of about 200 million Americans in the 1970s, “we were seen by 40-45 million people every single week,” he said. “There was no TiVo. There was no DVR. If you wanted to watch it, you had to watch it when it was on. That meant that you had a shared experience with 40 million people in America.” (Salam, 2023)

Opportunities

Although our discussion thus far may imply that recent shifts in our dominant communication medium have only downsides in terms of connectedness, we also hope these shifts present new-found opportunities for connection. In this final section, we offer several possibilities for prosocial outcomes that may be related to our contemporary and shifting media landscape.

As mentioned previously and has been noted by Meyrowitz, our new media-related communities formed on the basis of shared interests may allow for greater global communities and, hence, shared recognition of our common humanity. This speculation is similar to Peter Singer’s (1981) argument that our circles of moral concern have broadened over time to include not only people outside our immediate surroundings, but to nonhuman animals, all living beings, and to the planet and universe itself (see Waytz et al., 2019, for a discussion of the importance of political ideology on moral circles).

What role might media play in broadening our moral concerns and feelings of connectedness? In many respects, at a global level, media imply that we now have the ability to “know what others know” (at least somewhat), thereby enhancing our ability to perspective take or experience cognitive empathy with groups that have, in our recent pasts, been strangers or even perceived threats. Additionally, recently media have presented events and crises that were experienced throughout the world. In this sense, media may allow us to recognize that our affective response to world events are shared with others throughout the planet. Consistent with this argument, Newman (2023) speculated that over the last several years, levels of happiness (according to the World Happiness Report) that have remained rather stable reflect greater benevolence and kindness in response to global crises such as the COVID pandemic and police violence against people of color.

In addition to potentially increasing our ability to engage in cognitive empathy with a wider circle of diverse individuals, changes in media technologies may also provide additional opportunities for synchrony. First, though synchrony has typically been studied in faceto- face interpersonal contexts, researchers are increasingly studying how media stimuli can elicit the experience of synchrony (e.g., cortical activity during viewing a film, see Hasson et al., 2010). Of course, some types of media such as virtual reality (Tarr et al., 2018) or video conferencing (Nowak et al., 2017) afford approximations to interpersonal interaction, yet other scholars have suggested broadening the term synchrony to include interactions on social networks (e.g., social synchrony, De Choudhury et al., 2009).

In addition to synchronized behaviors between participants in a mediated context, media technologies also afford the opportunity to witness synchrony between others, often when the others may be geographically dispersed. For example, Playing for Change (https://www. playingforchange.com/) is an organization that orchestrates musical collaboration among artists from around the world who come together (digitally) to sing common songs, thereby inspiring and connecting not only the musicians themselves, but also the viewers who watch and listen to their songs. One of their first videos included musicians from such places as the U.S., the Netherlands, France, Russia, the Congo, Venezuela, and South Africa in their respective countries, but collaborating by singing Ben E. King’s song, “Stand by Me” (the video can be seen on YouTube at https://youtu.be/ Us-TVg40ExM?si=f-zSjbMHmKkSLvUE). This video has garnered over 200 million views in the last 15 years, with users expressing how the collaboration heightens their feeling of connectedness with other (e.g., “There’s just something so beautiful about the song and all the different people around the world coming together to make this,” “So beautiful, transformational, tears running down my face. We are all intricately connected in this world.”). Addition media projects that focus on synchronous behaviors from diverse groups include the 2010 movie Life in a Day (produced by Ridley Scott, directed by Kevin Macdonald) in which people throughout the world recorded themselves expressing what they love, hope, and fear on a specific date, July 24, 2010 (the trailer can be found on YouTube at https://youtu.be/ bT_UmBHMYzg?si=_siPpVUacFfPrAXg). Similar to Playing for Change, users’ posts reflect the feelings of connectedness that this film evokes (e.g., “Love this, hope that this helps people to see that we're really not all that different and that we all share the same fates and life on this planet,” “This is something everyone needs to watch to have a better understanding of how small and connected we really are and to have compassion for humanity.”).


CONCLUDING THOUGHTS

To summarize, we believe that changes in our media landscape present both challenges and opportunities to use media to enhance our connectedness and shared humanity. We selected Meyrowitz’s No Sense of Place as our hidden gem, not because it is hidden — indeed it has garnered much favorable attention and has spawned countless numbers of essays and programs of research. However, within the subfield of positive media psychology, this book has not garnered the attention that we believe it deserves. By taking stock of how shifting mediums alter our social lives and our shared realities, we hope that this essay will entice additional scholars to consider not only how media messages can enhance our sense of connectedness, but also how changes in dominant mediums can dampen or enhance our compassionate love for others.

Disclosure Statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author.

References

  • Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(2), 105–127. [https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760802650519]
  • Atherton, G., Sebanz, N., & Cross, L. (2019). Imagine all the synchrony: The effects of actual and imagined synchronous walking on attitudes towards marginalised groups. PLoS ONE, 14(5), e0216585. [https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216585]
  • Baimel, A., Birch, S. A. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2018). Coordinating bodies and minds: Behavioral synchrony fosters mentalizing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 281–290. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.008]
  • Basile, C., Lecce, S., & van Vugt, F. T. (2022). Synchrony during online encounters affects social affiliation and theory of mind but not empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 886639. [https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.886639]
  • Bowsher-Murray, C., Gerson, S., von dem Hagen, E., & Jones, C. R. G. (2022). The components of interpersonal synchrony in the typical population and in autism: A conceptual analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 897015. [https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.897015]
  • Brownstein, R. (2021, March 23). The show that changed television forever. The Atlantic. https://www.theatlantic.com/politics/archive/2021/03/how-all-family-changed-american-tv-forever/618353/
  • Cinelli, M., De Francisci Morales, G., Galeazzi, A., Quattrociocchi, W., & Starnini, M. (2021). The echo chamber effect on social media. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(9), e2023301118. [https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2023301118]
  • Crossey, B. P., Atherton, G., & Cross, L. (2021). Lost in the crowd: Imagining walking in synchrony with a crowd increases affiliation and deindividuation. PLoS ONE, 16(7), e0254017. [https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254017]
  • Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: A review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144-153. [https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466]
  • Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126. [https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.44.1.113]
  • Davis, M. H. (2017). Empathy, compassion, and social relationships. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon- Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compassion science (pp. 299-315). Oxford University Press. [https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464684.013.23]
  • Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47(1), 1-15. [https://doi.org/10.1086/268763]
  • Dayan, D., & Katz, E. (1992). Media events: The live broadcasting of history. Harvard University Press.
  • De Choudhury, M., Sundaram, H., John, A., & Seligmann, D. D. (2009). Social synchrony: Predicting mimicry of user actions in online social media. Proceedings of the International Conference on Computational Science and Engineering (pp. 151–158). IEEE. [https://doi.org/10.1109/CSE.2009.439]
  • Fischer, R., Callander, R., Reddish, P., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). How do rituals affect cooperation?: An experimental field study comparing nine ritual types. Human Nature, 24(2), 115–125. [https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9167-y]
  • Freeman, D., Aquino, K., & McFerran, B. (2009). Overcoming beneficiary race as an impediment to charitable donations: Social dominance orientation, the experience of moral elevation, and donation behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(1), 72–84. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208325415]
  • Haidt, J. (2003). Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 275–289). American Psychological Association. [https://doi.org/10.1037/10594-012]
  • Hasson, U., Malach, R., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(1), 40–48. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011]
  • Hobolt, S. B., Lawall, K., & Tilley, J. (2023). The polarizing effect of partisan echo chambers. American Political Science Review, 1-16. [https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055423001211]
  • Janicke, S. H., & Oliver, M. B. (2017). The relationship between elevation, connectedness, and compassionate love in meaningful films. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 6(3), 274–289. [https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000105]
  • Krämer, N., Eimler, S. C., Neubaum, G., Winter, S., Rösner, L., & Oliver, M. B. (2017). Broadcasting one world: How watching online videos can elicit elevation and reduce stereotypes. New Media & Society, 19(9), 1349-1368. [https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444816639963]
  • Lebow, S. (2022, December 21). How Gen Z consumes media in 5 charts. Insider Intelligence. https://www.insiderintelligence.com/content/how-gen-z-consumes-media-5-charts
  • Levy, J., & Feldman, R. (2019). Synchronous interactions foster empathy. Journal of Experimental Neuroscience, 13. [https://doi.org/10.1177/1179069519865799]
  • Levy, M. R., & Windahl, S. (1985). The concept of audience activity. In K. E. Rosengren, L. A. Wenner, & P. Palmgreen (Eds.), Media gratifications research: Current Perspectives (pp. 109–122). Sage.
  • Masta, K. E., & Worden, K. (2022, May 26). Local newspapers fact sheet. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/fact-sheet/local-newspapers/?tabId=tab-6a3b84c5-d51d-4c00-8164-fe7f522ea94e
  • McNeill, W. H. (1995). Keeping together in time: Dance and drill in human history. Harvard University Press.
  • Meyrowitz, J. (1985). No sense of place: The impact of electronic media on social behavior. Oxford University Press.
  • Meyrowitz, J. (1997). Shifting worlds of strangers: Medium theory and changes in “'them” versus “us.”. Sociological Inquiry, 67(1), 59–71.
  • Mitchell, A., Jurkowitz, M., Oliphant, J. B., & Shearer, E. (2021, February 22). How Americans navigated the news in 2020: A Tumultuous year in review: 1. About a quarter of republicans, democrats consistently turned only to news outlets whose audiences aligned with them politically in 2020. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2021/02/22/about-a-quarter-of-republicans-democrats-consistently-turned-only-to-news-outlets-whose-audiences-aligned-with-them-politically-in-2020/
  • Newman, K. M. (2023, March 21). World happiness report finds that crises make us kinder. Greater Good Magazine. https://greatergood.berkeley.edu/article/item/world_happiness_report_finds_that_crises_make_us_kinder
  • Nowak, K. L., Watt, J., & Walther, J. B. (2017). The influence of synchrony and sensory modality on the person perception process in computer-mediated groups. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 10(3), JCMC1038. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2005.tb00251.x]
  • Oliver, M. B. (1993). Exploring the paradox of the enjoyment of sad films. Human Communication Research, 19(3), 315–342. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1993.tb00304.x]
  • Oliver, M. B. (2008). Tender affective states as predictors of entertainment preference. Journal of Communication, 58(1), 40–61. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2007.00373.x]
  • Oliver, M. B., & Bartsch, A. (2010). Appreciation as audience response: Exploring entertainment gratifications beyond hedonism. Human Communication Research, 36(1), 53–81. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01368.x]
  • Oliver, M. B., Kim, K., Hoewe, J., Chung, M.-Y., Ash, E., Woolley, J. K., & Shade, D. D. (2015). Media-induced elevation as a means of enhancing feelings of intergroup connectedness. Journal of Social Issues, 71(1), 106–122. [https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12099]
  • Oliver, M. B., & Raney, A. A. (2011). Entertainment as pleasurable and meaningful: Identifying hedonic and eudaimonic motivations for entertainment consumption. Journal of Communication, 61(5), 984–1004. [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01585.x]
  • Paladino, M.-P., Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., & Schubert, T. W. (2010). Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1202– 1207. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610379234]
  • Pew Research Center. (2023, November 15). News platform Fact sheet. https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/fact-sheet/news-platform-fact-sheet/
  • Raney, A. A., Janicke, S. H., Oliver, M. B., Dale, K. R., Jones, R. P., & Cox, D. (2018). Profiling the audience for self-transcendent media: A national survey. Mass Communication and Society, 21(3), 296–319. [https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2017.1413195]
  • Raney, A. A., Janicke-Bowles, S. H., Oliver, M. B., & Dale, K. (2021). Introduction to positive media psychology. Routledge.
  • Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony a meta-analysis. Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie, 224(3), 168–189. [https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000252]
  • Ross Arguedas, A., Robertson, C., Fletcher, R., & Nielsen, R. (2022). Echo chambers, filter bubbles, and polarisation: A literature review. Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism. https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:6e357e97-7b16-450a-a827-a92c93729a08
  • Salam, M. (2023, December 6). Rob Reiner remembers Norman Lear: We’ve lost ‘a real champion of America’. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2023/12/06/arts/television/norman-lear-rob-reiner.html
  • Schnall, S., Roper, J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2010). Elevation leads to altruistic behavior. Psychological Science, 21(3), 315–320. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359882]
  • Singer, P. (1981). The expanding circle: Ethics, evolution, and moral progress. Princeton University Press.
  • Smith, A. (2006). Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and evolution. The Psychological Record, 56(1), 3–21.
  • Staub, E. (1990). Commentary on Part I. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 103–115). Cambridge University Press.
  • Stellar, J. E., Gordon, A. M., Piff, P. K., Cordaro, D., Anderson, C. L., Bai, Y., Maruskin, L. A., & Keltner, D. (2017). Self-transcendent emotions and their social functions: Compassion, gratitude, and awe bind us to others through prosociality. Emotion Review, 9(3), 200–207. [https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916684557]
  • Swarbrick, D., Bosnyak, D., Livingstone, S. R., Bansal, J., Marsh-Rollo, S., Woolhouse, M. H., & Trainor, L. J. (2019). How live music moves us: Head movement differences in audiences to live versus recorded music. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2682. [https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02682]
  • Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Silent disco: Dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(5), 343–349. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.02.004]
  • Tarr, B., Slater, M., & Cohen, E. (2018). Synchrony and social connection in immersive virtual reality. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 3693. [https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21765-4]
  • Vallone, R. P., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1985). The hostile media phenomenon: Biased perception and perceptions of media bias in coverage of the Beirut massacre. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(3), 577–585. [https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.577]
  • Waytz, A., Iyer, R., Young, L., Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2019). Ideological differences in the expanse of the moral circle. Nature Communications, 10, 4389. [https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-12227-0]
  • Wlodarczyk, A., Zumeta, L., Basabe, N., Rimé, B., & Páez, D. (2023). Religious and secular collective gatherings, perceived emotional synchrony and self-transcendent emotions: Two longitudinal studies. Current Psychology, 42(6), 4754–4771. [https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01826-0]
  • Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood, R. W., Vago, D. R., & Newberg, A. B. (2017). The varieties of self-transcendent experience. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 143–160. [https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000102]
  • Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2016). Empathy. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 871–884). Guilford Press.
  • Zickfeld, J. H., Schubert, T. W., Seibt, B., Blomster, J. K., Arriaga, P., Basabe, N., Blaut, A., Caballero, A., Carrera, P., Dalgar, I., Ding, Y., Dumont, K., Gaulhofer, V., Gracanin, A., Gyenis, R., Hu, C. P., Kardum, I., Lazarevic, L. B., Mathew, L., Mentser, S., Nussinson, R., Onuki, M., Paez, D., Pasztor, A., Peng, K., Petrovic, B., Pizarro, J. J., Schonefeld, V., Smieja, M., Tokaji, A., Vingerhoets, A., Vorster, A., Vuoskoski, J., Zhu, L., & Fiske, A. P. (2019). Kama muta: Conceptualizing and measuring the experience often labelled being moved across 19 nations and 15 languages. Emotion, 19(3), 402–424. [https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000450]
  • Zillmann, D. (2000). Mood management in the context of selective exposure theory. In M. E. Roloff (Ed.), Communication yearbook (Vol. 23, pp. 103–123). Sage.
  • Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1975). Viewer’s moral sanction of retribution in the appreciation of dramatic presentations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11(6), 572–582. [https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(75)90005-7]
  • Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1985). Affect, mood, and emotion as determinants of selective exposure. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 157– 190). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1986). Exploring the entertainment experience. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Perspectives on media effects (pp. 303–324). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
About the Authors

Dr. Mary Beth Oliver is the Bellisario Professor of Media Studies and Co-Director of the Media Effects Research Laboratory in the Bellisario College of Communications at Pennsylvania State University. Her research is in media psychology with an emphasis on viewers’ emotional and cognitive responses media, and to positive, self-transcendent media in particular. She is a Fellow and former President of the International Communication Association. She is co-editor of Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research (4th ed.), and has published in such journals as Communication Research, Journal of Communication, Human Communication Research, and Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, among others.

Yansheng Liu is a Ph.D. student at the Penn State Bellisario College of Communications. She studies the psychological effects of media and communication technology, especially how media can lead to prosocial attitudes and behavior.

Dr. Rebecca N. H. de Leeuw of the Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, is an assistant professor of Communication & Media. She devotes her research to the role of media in relation to well-being and feelings of connectedness. Rebecca is also fascinated by the question of how stories can nurture children’s social-emotional development, and how parents can strengthen these effects. Her work has been published in various top academic journals, including Media Psychology, Communication Research, and Journal of Communication. She also presented her work to the entertainment industry, including the Pixar Animation Studios.

Dr. Arthur A. Raney is a professor in the Department of Communication at the University at Buffalo. He studies how and why we enjoy and appreciate media entertainment, with specific interest in the roles that morality and moral emotions play in those processes. In recent years, he has spent much of his time examining how eudaimonic and self-transcendent media experiences can lead to beneficial outcomes and well-being. He has published four books and more than 100 journal articles, book chapters, and proceedings.

Dr. Enny Das is a professor of Communication and Persuasion at Radboud University and Director of the Centre for Language Studies. Her research focuses on how message features affect information processing and recipients’ responses to news and entertainment media and in interpersonal contexts. In recent years she has had a particular interest in the role of eudaimonic entertainment experiences in dealing with existential concerns such as the fear of death. Enny has published over 100 articles in communication and psychology journals such as the Journal of Communication, Psychological Science, and Health Communication. She is Associate Editor of the Journal of Media Psycholog y, and member of the Academic Europaea.

Dr. Marie-Louise Mares of University of Wisconsin- Madison is a professor in the Department of Communication Arts. Her research focuses on life-span developmental changes in media uses and responses, with an emphasis on prosocial, positive outcomes of media use. She is a Fellow of the International Communication Association, and an Associate Editor of Journal of Children and Media. She has published in such journals as Communication Research, Journal of Communication, Media Psychology, and Developmental Psychology, among others.